Dedre Gentner

Alice Gabrielle Twight Professor of Psychology & Education


Curriculum vitae



(847)467-1272


Department of Psychology

Northwestern University



Expertise Effects on Sorting Strategies of Causal Phenomena


Journal article


D. Gentner, Benjamin M. Rottman
2006

Semantic Scholar
Cite

Cite

APA   Click to copy
Gentner, D., & Rottman, B. M. (2006). Expertise Effects on Sorting Strategies of Causal Phenomena.


Chicago/Turabian   Click to copy
Gentner, D., and Benjamin M. Rottman. “Expertise Effects on Sorting Strategies of Causal Phenomena” (2006).


MLA   Click to copy
Gentner, D., and Benjamin M. Rottman. Expertise Effects on Sorting Strategies of Causal Phenomena. 2006.


BibTeX   Click to copy

@article{d2006a,
  title = {Expertise Effects on Sorting Strategies of Causal Phenomena},
  year = {2006},
  author = {Gentner, D. and Rottman, Benjamin M.}
}

Abstract

Expertise Effects on Sorting Strategies of Causal Phenomena Benjamin M. Rottman ([email protected]) Cognitive Science, Northwestern University 720 Clark Street #2N, Evanston, IL 60201 USA Dedre Gentner ([email protected]) Department of Psychology, Northwestern University Swift Hall 102, 2029 Sheridan Road, Evanston, IL 60208-2710 USA Table 1: Means and Standard Deviations for Expert and Novice Sortings Introduction Recent work has emphasized both the importance of relational categories in cognition, (Gentner & Kurtz, 2005) and role of causal structure in categorization of causal phenomena (Rehder, 2003; Sloman, Love, & Ahn, 1998). Whereas some categories may be defined primarily by features, relational information may be the sole determiner of other categories. Particularly, mechanism or the underlying structure which explains how a causal process functions (Forbus, 1984) seems to be a likely candidate for relational causal categories: the same type of mechanism can explain causal phenomena across many domains. Experiments The current study examines how participants categorize textual descriptions of real-world phenomena that vary along two dimensions: domain (e.g. economics) and causal structure (e.g. negative feedback). We compared novices (introductory psychology students) to experts (students in physics and an integrated science program)—students likely to have experience analyzing causal mechanisms. We predicted more causal sorting for experts than for novices. We created descriptions in a 5 (causal type) x 5 (domain) design. In writing the descriptions, we used Latent Semantic Analysis (LSA) (Landauer, Folts, & Laham, 1998) to ensure that the causal descriptions were relatively low in contextually relatedness. LSA relatedness ratings were significantly lower for descriptions within the same causal system (M=0.10, SD=0.04) than for those within the same domain (M=0.21, SD= 0.08), t(24)=5.36, p<.001. Participants were given 5 example cards, each distinct in causal type as well as in domain. They were asked to sort the remaining 20 descriptions into the five categories that were instantiated by the exemplar cards, plus one “Other” category. This task was designed to allow participants to sort either by domain or by causal system. Results and Discussion As predicted, experts sorted more cards into relational categories (matching the exemplar by causal system) than domain categories t(19)=2.10, p=.02 (1-tailed, paired). The novices sorted more cards by domain than by causal system t(19)=2.08, p=.03 (1-tailed, paired) (Table 1). Domain Match Causal Match Novices mean stdev Experts mean stdev Hierarchical cluster analysis (HCA) was conducted on the summed sorting data and revealed four groups based on domain for novices: novices conflated the domains of mechanical and electrical engineering. Five groups based on causal system were revealed for the experts. The sortings that deviated from the dominant sorting strategy could often be explained by exceptionally high LSA scores. The conflated mechanical/electrical engineering cluster received LSA ratings (M=.179, SD= .084) that were almost as high as the average within-domain pair (M=.209, SD=.084), t(26)=-.08, p=.22. Two out of three anomalies appearing in the HCA of the experts could be explained by exceptionally high LSA ratings. Apparently, extreme contextual relatedness drew experts to sort by domain despite a dominant causal system strategy. Acknowledgments Support was provided by a Northwestern Undergraduate Research Grant awarded to Ben Rottman and by NSF SBR- 95-11757 and ONR N00014-92-J-1095 to Dedre Gentner. References Forbus, K. D. (1984). Qualitative process theory. Journal of Artificial Intelligence, 24, 85-168. Gentner, D., & Kurtz, K. (2005). Learning and using relational categories. In W. K. Ahn, R. L, Goldstone, B. C. Love, A. B. Markman, & P. W. Wolff (Eds.), Categorization inside and outside the laboratory. Washington, DC: APA. Landauer, T. K., Foltz, P. W., & Laham, D. (1998). Introduction to Latent Semantic Analysis. Discourse Processes, 25, 259-284. Rehder, Bob. (2003). Categorization as causal reasoning. Cognitive Science, 27, 709-748. Sloman, S. A., Love, B. C., & Ahn, W-K. (1998). Feature centrality and conceptual coherence. Cognitive Science,


Share



Follow this website


You need to create an Owlstown account to follow this website.


Sign up

Already an Owlstown member?

Log in