Journal article
2003
Alice Gabrielle Twight Professor of Psychology & Education
(847)467-1272
Department of Psychology
Northwestern University
APA
Click to copy
Gentner, D., & Gunn, V. (2003). Structural Alignment Facilitates Discovering Differences.
Chicago/Turabian
Click to copy
Gentner, D., and V. Gunn. “Structural Alignment Facilitates Discovering Differences” (2003).
MLA
Click to copy
Gentner, D., and V. Gunn. Structural Alignment Facilitates Discovering Differences. 2003.
BibTeX Click to copy
@article{d2003a,
title = {Structural Alignment Facilitates Discovering Differences},
year = {2003},
author = {Gentner, D. and Gunn, V.}
}
What can differences tell us about the process of comparison? The structural alignment model posits that psychologically salient differences arise out of commonalities (Markman & Gentner, 1993) . This leads to the counterintuitive prediction that high similarity pairs (e .g ., bicycle/tricycle) should elicit more differences than low similarity pairs (e .g ., broom/ambulance), since high similarity pairs have more commonalities from which to derive differences, Gentner and Markman (1994) tested this prediction using a speeded difference task . Participants were asked to list one difference for as many word pairs (of high and low similarity) as possible in 5 minutes . As predicted, participants listed a difference for more high similarity pairs than low similarity pairs . The authors concluded that the high similarity pairs have a difference advantage because they have a larger common system from which to derive differences. However, another possible explanation is that many high similarity pairs (e .g ., hotel/motel) have been compared in the past, resulting in a stock of pre-stored differences . Experiment I addresses this alternative by directly testing whether producing commonalities facilitates listing differences . Forty-eight participants first listed a commonality for high and low similarity pairs . They were then given a speeded difference task, as described above . Half of the pairs were "old" (i .e ., used in the commonality task), and half were "new ." Differences were produced for more old pairs (M=5 .9) than new pairs (M=5 .4, p< .04), as predicted . This effect was obtained despite the presence of word pairs that were unlikely to have pre-stored differences (low similarity) . However, we still needed to rule out other explanationse.g .,, that the difference facilitation was a general result of recent co-activation of the two terms . In Experiment 2, half of the 48 participants performed a commonality task and the other half performed a thematic connection task on the same word pairs (which where chosen to facilitate either task) . For example, the pair "tree/child" could elicit the commonality "both grow" or the thematic connection "a child climbs a tree ." After this setting task, all participants were given the speeded difference task . Sample responses are presented in Table 1 . The results showed the predicted interaction (p= .004): for the Commonality group, differences were easier to list